Reduced attention to human eyes in autism-associated Shank3 mutant laboratory beagle dogs

Abstract

Autistic individuals often exhibit reduced attention to faces and eyes, which may underlie their social difficulty. This study used eye-tracking techniques to explore visual attention towards faces in Shank3 mutant laboratory beagle dogs, a model for autism, to identify parallels with human autism. We first assessed visual attention differences towards the eyes between Shank3 mutant and wild-type (WT) laboratory beagles by presenting them with human and dog face images. Then, using the gaze cueing paradigm, we directed the dogs’ gaze towards the eyes and mouth and quantified their gaze shifts. Finally, we investigated the impact of oxytocin on eye-gaze behavior by comparing gaze patterns under pre-administration, vehicle, and oxytocin conditions while viewing human faces. We found that mutant dogs showed a reduced proportional viewing time of human eyes than WT dogs (p = 0.032), but no difference in proportional eye viewing time when viewing dog faces (p = 0.691). Mutant dogs shifted their gazes away from the human eyes more quickly than the mouth (p = 0.043), unlike WT dogs (p = 0.345), suggesting an active eye avoidance. Furthermore, exogenous oxytocin increased proportional viewing time on human eyes in mutant dogs than pre-administration and vehicle conditions (p = 0.022), suggesting a potential effect of oxytocin on social attention in autism. To our knowledge, this study is the first to report an eye avoidance phenotype in an animal model of autism. These findings contribute to our understanding of the mechanisms underlying social difficulties in autism and the development of supporting strategies for autism.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Design of an eye-tracking system for dogs.
Fig. 2: Shank3 mutant dogs showed a reduced proportional viewing time in the eye region of human faces.
Fig. 3: Shank3 mutant dogs showed a typical proportional viewing time in the eye region of dog faces.
Fig. 4: Shank3 mutant dogs shifted away from the eye region more quickly than the mouth region.
Fig. 5: Nasal spray of oxytocin increased the proportional viewing time in the human eye region in Shank3 mutant dogs.

Data availability

The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

References

  1. Association AP. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.


    Google Scholar
     

  2. Adolphs R, Sears L, Piven J. Abnormal processing of social information from faces in autism. Autism. London: Routledge; 2013. p. 126–134.

  3. Klin A, Jones W, Schultz R, Volkmar F, Cohen D. Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59:809–16.

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  4. Emery NJ. The eyes have it: the neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2000;24:581–604.

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  5. Stuart N, Whitehouse A, Palermo R, Bothe E, Badcock N. Eye gaze in autism spectrum disorder: a review of neural evidence for the eye avoidance hypothesis. J Autism Dev Disord. 2023;53:1884–905.

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  6. Tanaka JW, Sung A. The “eye avoidance” hypothesis of autism face processing. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46:1538–52.

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  7. Chevallier C, Kohls G, Troiani V, Brodkin ES, Schultz RT. The social motivation theory of autism. Trends Cognit Sci. 2012;16:231–9.


    Google Scholar
     

  8. Moriuchi JM, Klin A, Jones W. Mechanisms of diminished attention to eyes in autism. Am J Psychiatry. 2017;174:26–35.

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  9. Xu D, Zhi Y, Liu X, Guan L, Yu J, Zhang D, et al. WDR62-deficiency causes autism-like behaviors independent of microcephaly in mice. Neurosci Bull. 2023;39:1333–47.

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  10. Kaiser T, Zhou Y, Feng G. Animal models for neuropsychiatric disorders: prospects for circuit intervention. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2017;45:59–65.

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  11. de la Torre-Ubieta L, Won H, Stein JL, Geschwind DH. Advancing the understanding of autism disease mechanisms through genetics. Nat Med. 2016;22:345–61.

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  12. Del Pino I, Rico B, Marín O. Neural circuit dysfunction in mouse models of neurodevelopmental disorders. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2018;48:174–82.

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  13. Sato M, Nakai N, Fujima S, Choe KY, Takumi T. Social circuits and their dysfunction in autism spectrum disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2023;28:3194–206.

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  14. Kelly E, Meng F, Fujita H, Morgado F, Kazemi Y, Rice LC, et al. Regulation of autism-relevant behaviors by cerebellar–prefrontal cortical circuits. Nat Neurosci. 2020;23:1102–10.

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  15. Jennings CG, Landman R, Zhou Y, Sharma J, Hyman J, Movshon JA, et al. Opportunities and challenges in modeling human brain disorders in transgenic primates. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19:1123–30.

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  16. Zhao H, Jiang YH, Zhang YQ. Modeling autism in non-human primates: Opportunities and challenges. Autism Res. 2018;11:686–94.

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  17. Persson ME, Sundman A-S, Halldén L-L, Trottier AJ, Jensen P. Sociality genes are associated with human-directed social behaviour in golden and Labrador retriever dogs. PeerJ. 2018;6:e5889.

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  18. Persson ME, Wright D, Roth LS, Batakis P, Jensen P. Genomic regions associated with interspecies communication in dogs contain genes related to human social disorders. Sci Rep. 2016;6:33439.

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  19. Topál J, Román V, Turcsán B. The dog (Canis familiaris) as a translational model of autism: It is high time we move from promise to reality. Wiley Interdiscip Reviews: Cognit Sci. 2019;10:e1495.


    Google Scholar
     

  20. Galambos Á, Petró E, Nagy B, Turcsán B, Topál J. The effects of social and non-social distracting stimuli on dogs with different levels of social competence–Empirical evidence for a canine model of autism. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2021;244:105451.


    Google Scholar
     

  21. Somppi S, Törnqvist H, Hänninen L, Krause C, Vainio O. Dogs do look at images: eye tracking in canine cognition research. Anim Cogn. 2012;15:163–74.

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  22. Somppi S, Törnqvist H, Hänninen L, Krause CM, Vainio O. How dogs scan familiar and inverted faces: an eye movement study. Anim Cogn. 2014;17:793–803.

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  23. Virányi Z, Topál J, Gácsi M, Miklósi Á, Csányi V. Dogs respond appropriately to cues of humans’ attentional focus. Behavioural Process. 2004;66:161–72.


    Google Scholar
     

  24. Gácsi M, Miklósi Á, Varga O, Topál J, Csányi V. Are readers of our face readers of our minds? Dogs (Canis familiaris) show situation-dependent recognition of human’s attention. Anim Cognition. 2004;7:144–53.


    Google Scholar
     

  25. Hare B, Call J, Tomasello M. Communication of food location between human and dog (Canis familiaris). Evolution Commun. 1998;2:137–59.


    Google Scholar
     

  26. Miklösi Á, Polgárdi R, Topál J, Csányi V. Use of experimenter-given cues in dogs. Anim Cognition. 1998;1:113–21.


    Google Scholar
     

  27. Moessner R, Marshall CR, Sutcliffe JS, Skaug J, Pinto D, Vincent J, et al. Contribution of SHANK3 mutations to autism spectrum disorder. Am J Hum Genet. 2007;81:1289–97.

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  28. Boccuto L, Lauri M, Sarasua SM, Skinner CD, Buccella D, Dwivedi A, et al. Prevalence of SHANK3 variants in patients with different subtypes of autism spectrum disorders. Eur J Hum Genet. 2013;21:310–6.

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  29. Tian R, Li Y, Zhao H, Lyu W, Zhao J, Wang X et al. Modeling SHANK3-associated autism spectrum disorder in Beagle dogs via CRISPR/Cas9 gene editing. Mol Psychiatry. 2023;28:3739–3750.

  30. Ren W, Huang K, Li Y, Yang Q, Wang L, Guo K et al. Altered pupil responses to social and non-social stimuli in Shank3 mutant dogs. Mol Psychiatry. 2023;28:3751–3759.

  31. Téglás E, Gergely A, Kupán K, Miklósi Á, Topál J. Dogs’ gaze following is tuned to human communicative signals. Curr Biol. 2012;22:209–12.

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  32. Somppi S, Törnqvist H, Topál J, Koskela A, Hänninen L, Krause CM, et al. Nasal oxytocin treatment biases dogs’ visual attention and emotional response toward positive human facial expressions. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1854.

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  33. Kliemann D, Dziobek I, Hatri A, Steimke R, Heekeren HR. Atypical reflexive gaze patterns on emotional faces in autism spectrum disorders. J Neurosci. 2010;30:12281–7.

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  34. Le J, Kou J, Zhao W, Fu M, Zhang Y, Becker B, et al. Oxytocin facilitation of emotional empathy is associated with increased eye gaze toward the faces of individuals in emotional contexts. Front Neurosci. 2020;14:803.

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  35. Nagasawa M, Mitsui S, En S, Ohtani N, Ohta M, Sakuma Y, et al. Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. Science. 2015;348:333–6.

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  36. Nagasawa M, Ogawa M, Mogi K, Kikusui T. Intranasal oxytocin treatment increases eye-gaze behavior toward the owner in ancient Japanese dog breeds. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1624.

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  37. Yi L, Wang Q, Song C, Han ZR. Hypo‐or hyperarousal? The mechanisms underlying social information processing in autism. Child Dev Perspect. 2022;16:215–22.


    Google Scholar
     

  38. Auyeung B, Lombardo MV, Heinrichs M, Chakrabarti B, Sule A, Deakin JB, et al. Oxytocin increases eye contact during a real-time, naturalistic social interaction in males with and without autism. Transl Psychiatry. 2015;5:e507–e507.

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  39. Gordon I, Vander Wyk BC, Bennett RH, Cordeaux C, Lucas MV, Eilbott JA, et al. Oxytocin enhances brain function in children with autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2013;110:20953–8.

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  40. Karl S, Boch M, Virányi Z, Lamm C, Huber L. Training pet dogs for eye-tracking and awake fMRI. Behav Res Methods. 2020;52:838–56.

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  41. Gong X, Huang Y-X, Wang Y, Luo Y-J. Revision of the chinese facial affective picture system. Chin Ment Health J. 2011;25:40–46.


    Google Scholar
     

  42. Wang Y, Luo Y-J. Standardization and assessment of college students’ facial expression of emotion. Chin J Clin Psychol. 2005;13:396–8.

    CAS 

    Google Scholar
     

  43. Kis A, Kanizsár O, Gácsi M, Topál J. Intranasally administered oxytocin decreases heart rate and increases heart rate variability in dogs. J Veterinary Behavior: Clin Appl Res. 2014;9:e15.


    Google Scholar
     

  44. Kovács K, Kis A, Kanizsár O, Hernádi A, Gácsi M, Topál J. The effect of oxytocin on biological motion perception in dogs (Canis familiaris). Anim Cognition. 2016;19:513–22.


    Google Scholar
     

  45. Light KC, Grewen KM, Amico JA. More frequent partner hugs and higher oxytocin levels are linked to lower blood pressure and heart rate in premenopausal women. Biol Psychol. 2005;69:5–21.

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  46. Lawson EA, Marengi DA, DeSanti RL, Holmes TM, Schoenfeld DA, Tolley CJ. Oxytocin reduces caloric intake in men. Obesity. 2015;23:950–6.

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  47. Kemp AH, Quintana DS, Kuhnert R-L, Griffiths K, Hickie IB, Guastella AJ. Oxytocin increases heart rate variability in humans at rest: implications for social approach-related motivation and capacity for social engagement. PLoS One. 2012;7:e44014.

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  48. Schlegelmilch K, Wertz AE. Visual segmentation of complex naturalistic structures in an infant eye-tracking search task. PLoS One. 2022;17:e0266158.

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  49. Gharib A, Thompson BL. Analysis and novel methods for capture of normative eye-tracking data in 2.5-month old infants. PLoS One. 2022;17:e0278423.

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  50. Wang Q, Li X, Gong X, Yin T, Liu Q, Yi L et al. Autistic children’s visual sensitivity to face movement. Dev Psychopathol. 2023;36:1616–1625.

  51. Maris E, Oostenveld R. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG-and MEG-data. J Neurosci Methods. 2007;164:177–90.

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  52. Gutkowska J, Jankowski M, Mukaddam-Daher S, McCann S. Oxytocin is a cardiovascular hormone. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2000;33:625–33.

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  53. Cuve HC, Gao Y, Fuse A. Is it avoidance or hypoarousal? A systematic review of emotion recognition, eye-tracking, and psychophysiological studies in young adults with autism spectrum conditions. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2018;55:1–13.


    Google Scholar
     

  54. Kliemann D, Dziobek I, Hatri A, Baudewig J, Heekeren HR. The role of the amygdala in atypical gaze on emotional faces in autism spectrum disorders. J Neurosci. 2012;32:9469–76.

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  55. Mathersul D, McDonald S, Rushby JA. Autonomic arousal explains social cognitive abilities in high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. Int J Psychophysiol. 2013;89:475–82.

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  56. Wang Q, Lu L, Zhang Q, Fang F, Zou X, Yi L. Eye avoidance in young children with autism spectrum disorder is modulated by emotional facial expressions. J Abnorm Psychol. 2018;127:722.

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  57. Dalton KM, Nacewicz BM, Johnstone T, Schaefer HS, Gernsbacher MA, Goldsmith HH, et al. Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:519–26.

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  58. Siniscalchi M, d’Ingeo S, Minunno M, Quaranta A. Communication in dogs. Animals. 2018;8:131.

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  59. Horowitz A. Attention to attention in domestic dog (Canis familiaris) dyadic play. Anim Cogn. 2009;12:107–18.

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  60. van der Borg JA, Schilder MB, Vinke CM, de Vries H. Dominance in domestic dogs: a quantitative analysis of its behavioural measures. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0133978.

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar
     

  61. Correia-Caeiro C, Guo K, Mills D. Bodily emotional expressions are a primary source of information for dogs, but not for humans. Anim Cognition. 2021;24:267–79.


    Google Scholar
     

  62. Wang G-D, Zhai W, Yang H-C, Fan R-X, Cao X, Zhong L, et al. The genomics of selection in dogs and the parallel evolution between dogs and humans. Nat Commun. 2013;4:1860.

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  63. Rossano F, Nitzschner M, Tomasello M. Domestic dogs and puppies can use human voice direction referentially. Proc R Soc B: Biol Sci. 2014;281:20133201.


    Google Scholar
     

  64. Domes G, Heinrichs M, Michel A, Berger C, Herpertz SC. Oxytocin improves “mind-reading” in humans. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:731–3.

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  65. Menon R, Neumann ID. Detection, processing and reinforcement of social cues: regulation by the oxytocin system. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2023;24:761–77.

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  66. Le J, Zhang L, Zhao W, Zhu S, Lan C, Kou J, et al. Infrequent intranasal oxytocin followed by positive social interaction improves symptoms in autistic children: a pilot randomized clinical trial. Psychother Psychosom. 2022;91:335–47.

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  67. McEwen BS. Early life influences on life‐long patterns of behavior and health. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2003;9:149–54.

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

  68. Claessens SE, Daskalakis NP, van der Veen R, Oitzl MS, de Kloet ER, Champagne DL. Development of individual differences in stress responsiveness: an overview of factors mediating the outcome of early life experiences. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2011;214:141–54.

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar
     

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported in part by by grants from the National Key Research and Development Program (2021ZD0203901 to Y.Q.Z.), the National Natural Science Foundation of China (32394030 to Y.Q.Z., 32200872 to Q.D.W., and 32271116 to L.Y.), Wuhan Municipal S&T Project (Grant No. 2024020702030125 to Y.Q.Z.), and the Canine Biobank, Chinese Academy of Sciences (KFJ-BRP-004 to Y.Q.Z.).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

LY and YQZ conceptualized the project, supervised data collection and analysis. YML, QDW and SQY designed and performed experiments. YML, QDW and LTK completed data collection and analysis. YML, QDW, LY, and YQZ wrote the manuscript, and KG, LY, and YQZ finalized the manuscript. YML and QDW contributed equally to this work as first contributing authors. LY and YQZ contributed equally to this work as supervisors.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to
Yong Q. Zhang or Li Yi.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was conducted in full compliance with established guidelines for animal experimentation and received ethical approval from the Ethics Committee of the Institute of Genetics and Developmental Biology, Chinese Academy of Sciences (AP2022001).

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

41380_2025_2965_MOESM1_ESM.pdf

Supplementary figure 1 (Fig.S1), Supplementary figure 2 (Fig.S2), Supplementary table 1 (Table S1), Supplementary table 2 (Table S2)

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Li, Y., Wang, Q., Yuan, S. et al. Reduced attention to human eyes in autism-associated Shank3 mutant laboratory beagle dogs.
Mol Psychiatry (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-025-02965-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-025-02965-7

The Favicon for the website, dogsandpurses(dot)com, features an all-black background with a minimalist line drawing of a puppy's head poking out of a stylish purse. The puppy's head is drawn with a cute and friendly expression, making it the focal point of the design. The purse, which the puppy is emerging from, is depicted with clean, elegant lines. The contrast between the black background and the white line drawing creates a striking and modern look for the Favicon.
Dogs and Purses Favicon

WANT MORE?

SIGN UP TO RECEIVE THE LATEST on PAWS and PURSES in PERFECT PROPORTION.

We don’t spam! Read our privacy policy for more info.

This post was originally published on this site be sure to check out more of their content.